Neuroscientist Explains Memory in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Released on 11/11/2021
Hi, my name is Daphna Shohamy.
I'm a neuroscientist at Columbia University.
And today I've been asked to explain memory
at five levels of increasing complexity.
My research aims to understand how memories are created
and how they shape who we are,
what we do, and the decisions we make.
[gentle music]
We're here today to talk about memory.
When I say the word memory what comes to your mind?
Like I once went on vacation to Dominican Republic.
I think I was like six or five years old.
So on your trip what was the funnest day ever?
There was this pool, I love pools.
And then there's a slide you go down
and there's pool noodles in there too.
And then there's a little tiny sandbox.
It's pretty amazing, right?
Because it happened like a year or two ago.
Our memory is basically a record in our brain
of something that happened in the past,
but that record it created isn't perfect.
So that day, do you remember was the pool at your hotel?
I don't really remember.
Do you remember from that day what color towel you used
when you got out of the pool?
I think the one I used was a dark blue or a light blue or,
or like sort of a teal.
When you were telling me about the sandbox,
it felt like you didn't have to make that guess,
that you could just see the sandbox in your eyes.
I asked you about the towel.
It doesn't come to mind immediately,
but you can stop and think about it
and kind of make a good guess because there are things
you know about the world.
And for those of us who study memory
that's a really strong hint as to how memory works.
And so for you your memory kept all the fun stuff
and all the stuff that was important to you,
but the details like did you have to have a ticket
to get in or how you got there,
all these other less interesting, less exciting aspects,
your brain kind of decided letting go
of all that information.
I think as you get older you start forgetting things more
because you have to make space for new things,
which is why you forget stuff.
Yeah, that's a great insight.
Basically we can't remember everything.
So when you tell us this memory of that vacation
and your brain is able to kind of play almost this movie
of a memory, a moment, a day that you had in your life
a while ago, where do those memories come from?
I do know that there's different parts of the brain.
So there's probably a part of your brain
that remembers a bunch of stuff.
The hippocampus is a part of your brain that if you
kind of took it out and looked at it,
it looks a lot like a seahorse
and it plays a really important role in creating those kinds
of memories that help you two years after your vacation
share with me what happened that day,
all these details of that day.
There's another part of the brain.
It's actually also important for memory
and it's called the amygdala and it is important for keeping
all the kind of emotional processing going for memory.
So memory for things that are really scary,
it's gonna talk to the hippocampus now
and get that memory to be really strong.
Why do you think it might be important
to remember scary things?
Let's say you, you accidentally cut yourself.
Then your brain makes a note
and says don't get too close to sharp things,
or you're going to cut yourself again.
You got it.
And so we don't just remember everything
and we don't remember random things.
We remember the things that matter to us the most.
So Abigail based on our conversation today,
can you tell me what is a memory?
A memory is something stored in your hippocampus
and your hippocampus is a part of your brain.
It's just a big record.
And sometimes you can forget parts of the record
that's not that important to you.
You got it.
[gentle music]
What do you know about memory?
I know when you see something you could like
kind of like picture it,
like in your head you can imagine it.
I remember yesterday I got like orange juice,
but let's say like a year or two, you might think like, oh,
that day I got water.
And on one hand memory is like a record of something
that happened in the past that we can carry with us
and we can like bring it back to mind.
And on the other hand we need to be a little bit suspicious
sometimes because we might get it wrong.
In what world would that memory of the orange juice
maybe be useful?
Let's say you grew up in a place that the orange juice
is just meh, and then you go on vacation going somewhere
and then it might change how you feel about orange juice.
Sometimes, you know, memory is doing something
much simpler but no less important for us,
which is it's helping us figure out what's good
and what's bad.
And if we can remember what's good
and what's bad from what we did in the past,
that can help us make decisions about what to do next time.
So Dylan, you may be surprised to learn this,
but we asked your mom what some of your
favorite candies are.
So you have to make a decision between these two candies
and whichever one you pick,
you actually get to take with you.
I wish I could say both,
but I guess I'll go with pixie sticks.
There you go, there is your decision.
Okay, you can take those.
It took you some time there.
You looked like you were really working hard
at that problem.
And that's interesting because actually we know that from
research that when people have to make a decision between
two things they like equally,
some people like economists think oh those are the easiest
decisions because they're both good options.
But as psychologists and neuroscientists we know
it's the opposite.
One of the reasons is because there's no simple answer of
like I know that one's better I'm just gonna go with it.
And so we think that that's the kind of decision where
memory is especially important because you have to kind of
come up with more information.
Exactly, and then also like after it was like,
maybe like I would've, enjoyed this more than that
and like maybe I should've chose the other one.
And you don't know.
Everything you just said is exactly
what we know from research.
All the thoughts running through your mind,
were really at kind of about a prediction
into the future, right?
You're like, how, how will this taste if,
when it's in my mouth or how will I feel about my decision
down the road?
You know we think of memory as something in the past,
but it's an example of how you use your memories of these
two candies to predict what's gonna happen in the future
so that you can make a decision in the present.
But you did it, you made your way through that torture.
And now you've got to keep the candy.
Am I always gonna like choose pixie sticks
or sour patch kids?
What's gonna like change that?
We fluctuate a lot because we use different kinds of memory
to kind of resolve the uncertainty basically every time.
But also the way we make decisions will change
because our memories change.
What's interesting about your brain as a teenager is that we
know it's actually a phase of life where the brain is
especially sensitive to rewards,
to things that are exciting and positive.
It's a phase of life where those rewards,
whether it's candy or your friends or whatever it is
that's exciting can have an especially powerful control
over the decisions you make and the memories you create.
[gentle music]
Is there any particular memory for you that you feel has
been either kind of influential in your,
in deciding which areas to pursue or that you feel is sort
of emblematic of what it is to have a memory?
I was probably six years old
and my aunt who had stage four ovarian cancer,
she battled it for 20 years.
She got me my first science kit and she asked me to cure
cancer and I will never forget running into my aunt's room
and just opening up the box and seeing that microscope,
it was a tiny little blue one
and the little microscope slides and the little pipettes.
So they just, that memory will never be forgotten
my whole life.
I've got one too.
I was nine and we were at the science expo in San Francisco.
Two scientists were dissecting an eye, a cow's eye.
And I was like that is the coolest thing I've ever seen.
And something about that that carried me forward.
Something happened so long ago,
it left some long lasting trace through neural circuits
and it continues to shape the decisions we make
about what to study and what to do.
Just being able to apply those memories in the future
is just so crucial in everyday life of the humans
and the fact that we're learning how that works.
And I would love to hear more about your side of memory
and the mechanisms that you're studying.
So we're interested in the idea that memory is kind of
a pervasive force that shapes all our behaviors.
And we're trying to understand how different kinds of memory
are organized in different structures of the brain.
And then to understand how those different structures work
together to orchestrate complex cognitive behaviors
like decision-making or reasoning and thinking.
So in your research or you focus more on implicit
or explicit memory?
My work has actually kind of pushed against
that distinction between memories that kind of are
consciously accessible versus unconscious.
When you say that you don't necessarily look at implicit
and explicit memory as anything different,
if you were to take a step back,
what would you define as implicit and explicit memory?
The best way to kind of think about that distinction
really goes back historically to one of the most important
discoveries in memory research.
The patient was referred to famously as patient HM.
The neurosurgeon went in and removed the tissue that
happened to be right around the hippocampus
on both the left side and the right side of HM's brain.
But then they started noticing something odd
in his behavior.
He was not able to create new memories of the experiences
he had after the surgery.
And that led Brenda Milner and her colleagues to report that
the hippocampus was very important for memory,
but a one particular kind, these sort of explicit,
or as now referred to them episodic memories.
But the hippocampus was not necessary for learning skills
like mirror tracing,
things that you can't necessarily articulate
but you just get better at over time.
And it really led to a couple of decades or more even
of an enormous amount of very important work that kind
of kept on breaking memory down further
and further into different types.
Episodic and semantic as both forms of explicit memory
where episodic refers to memory for an event that happened
like what you did yesterday morning
and semantic refers to general knowledge about the world.
Implicit memory is being broken down into a bunch of
different kinds like skills or habits or conditioning.
And when I started graduate school,
many of us felt kind of the next question was really to
understand how do we now understand how they work together.
This was right around when functional magnetic resonance
imaging started becoming a popular tool
for measuring brain activity.
We could scan the entire brain and we could ask questions
about multiple brain regions at once.
And we discovered that what we might expect
during a skill learning task that only the striatum
might show activity,
that we also saw the activity in the hippocampus.
Or when we asked someone to form an episodic memory
that we thought might depend only on the hippocampus
we suddenly also saw activity in the prefrontal cortex.
And so the confluence of these new ideas and questions about
how different forms of memory interact
together with the development of new tools for studying
the human mind and brain allowed us to kind of adjust
our view of memory systems,
to think of them less as multiple completely separate
independent systems and instead to try to understand how
they really work in concert with each other
and give rise to all kinds of behaviors that might not fit
neatly into one category or another.
Are the connectivities that you're seeing using the FMRI
in your studies, are they different when you look at
the implicit and explicit memories?
Are you seeing more activity in the striatum in comparison
to the hippocampus or the prefrontal cortex?
Yeah, you know things are kind of breaking down
in unexpected ways I'd say.
I think there's been a lot of really great work out of
multiple labs showing hippocampal activity related to
behaviors that don't look anything like episodic memories,
but which might depend on episodic memories right?
So for example also when you're making a decision about how
to choose between two good options
that suddenly you might see activity in the hippocampus
related to the choice itself,
others have found that you find activity in the hippocampus
not only when people are forming memories,
but also when they're imagining events that are
gonna happen in the future.
So it kind of forced us to rethink the way we define what
the hippocampus might be doing in a way that can account for
creating memories, thinking about the future,
making decisions and other kinds of behaviors that involve
what we would refer to you in the field
as relational processing.
And that raises all kinds of new questions about
what memory really is.
Reminiscing about our early influences that made us
want to be scientists is that, you know,
of course we know, we know better than to think that those
are necessarily accurate memories.
You know this reminds me of sort of the
classic example from literature when people talk about
memory of course is Marcel Proust's
Madeleine in Remembrance of Things Past.
Where in the book it's this taste for the,
for the protagonist this taste of the crumbs of
the Madeleine cookie and the tea that bring him back
to his childhood.
You know, seven volumes then emerge of memories of,
of that childhood.
Digging further into earlier drafts of Proust's novel
that initially it didn't describe a Madeleine at all.
It's very interesting to see the imperfections
of the human brain and being able to rewrite something
that happened to you and fully believe it yourself.
I think that that's one of the coolest errors
and faults in the human brain that could exist.
You know I think these imperfections,
I interpret them as an indication that the role of memory
is much less about being accurate representation of the past
and much more about being kind of a flexible compass
into the future.
[gentle music]
So what are you studying these days?
Well, I'm preparing my dissertation.
The general topic is going to be how we perceive
different types of social stimuli,
like different people's faces and make judgements about them
and how the way that we report on what we are experiencing
tells us about what's actually going on inside our heads.
What I love about what you were describing is like
you didn't use the word memory,
were how you were talking about kind of perceptions
and social judgments but I think probably we'd both agree
that memory is a big part of that.
Oh of course.
Right, like it reminds me so much of, you know,
what we in the field would refer to as statistical learning.
On one hand we might have like a one-shot memory,
a memory of something that we saw yesterday,
like what we did or what we ate or where we were.
But a lot of our memories and our knowledge
instead are based on many, many, many experiences.
Oh, absolutely.
I think this sort of reminds me of another project
that I'm doing.
It's fairly easy to get different participants to experience
consistent negative emotions to the same stimuli,
but with positive emotions it's so much harder to study.
I might not react the same to like this like cute cat video
than someone else.
And I definitely believe that people's memory
that they're bringing up unconsciously
when they're experiencing something to make
an emotional judgment informs especially our experience
with positive emotion possibly more than negative emotion.
Some of those examples really remind me also some of the
questions about kind of abnormalities in,
in memory or trauma or disruptions in memory
and how they also play out in terms of not just
what people remember but what they do with
those memories right?
As far as the way that my research plays into it
is like the first step in making a judgment
about what to do based on an experience we're calling from
memory is deciding what your emotional response was.
I think what we're still kind of trying to understand
as a field is the more kind of detailed
and bigger picture of where does this model live?
How does it get updated based on experiences?
Why does that sometimes happen and sometimes not?
There are actually people who are born
without a hippocampus.
They have disruptions to episodic memories.
They have trouble after this conversation remembering
what the conversation was about,
but they do have pretty good semantic information.
They have a good sense of knowledge about the world.
And that's interesting for two reasons.
One, it shows this association between the role of the
hippocampus and these two forms of memory,
episodic memories and semantic memory.
The other reason it's interesting is that people often
assume that we get to semantic memory
through episodic memory,
that we encounter one dog and then another dog,
and then another dog and then we learned the concept,
the semantic notion of a dog.
And these people, their profile suggests that you can
learn semantic information.
You can learn general knowledge about the world,
even without ever having the capacity
to build those individual memories.
In people with intact brains,
you might use a particular process,
but if that brain region that subserves that process
is damaged, that it's not that you can't do it anymore
you just now do it a different way.
Right.
It's really hard to tell people like,
just don't use your memory to do this.
And we just sort of have to infer
and guess at what strategies that they're doing.
Is there's a particular research question that the answer
you think is gonna inform the research in your lab
and perhaps in the rest of your field?
You know when I look back at,
some of the work for my own lab and in the field in general,
I really feel like some of the most exciting discoveries
were not an answer to a question that was around before,
but they were discoveries that made us realize
we weren't asking the right question.
I think one example of that even has to do with the
connection between memory and decision-making.
There was a discovery about how the striatum responds to
rewards and to learning that just all of a sudden flipped
the way we thought about the role of reward
and learning and memory,
and may force us to realize there was something
fundamentally connected between these processes.
And that discovery just raised a whole new set of questions
that didn't exist before.
Yeah, I guess then the trick is we need to be designing
studies that will make us most likely to find those sort of
unexpected things, which is kind of funny.
It's like how do you look for something that you don't know
what it is?
Each project should be very focused and rigorous
and know what a study is designed for.
But at the same time we have to keep our minds open,
our eyes open for what else is happening.
Some of the most interesting discoveries
didn't make sense at first.
As someone who is studying social phenomena
I like to hear that.
There you go, exactly.
[gentle music]
Thanks so much for coming, it's great to see you.
It's been a while.
It has been awhile, I'm really excited to be here.
It'd be great to just start by talking about your work
at the broadest level.
To my mind your work has really revolutionized the field
of cognitive neuroscience more broadly.
I've been really interested in how past understandings
of the brain have really focused on,
on pinpointing exactly what each piece of the brain does.
But I think that there's also a broader conversation
happening in the brain,
which is between one region and another.
So it's actually the pattern of connections between these
regions that would allow for the flow of information.
But I think the tools that have become widely available now
from the physics community and mathematics
and computer science are under the umbrella of,
of network science.
So that is a science of networks,
a science of understanding how bits of a system are
interconnected with one another.
Not just comparing two groups of people
or two kinds of two species,
but we can ask even within a single person,
how does the pattern of connectivity in my brain change
as we talk with one another?
How does the information flow change?
I think a lot about traffic on roadway networks
as a good example of this.
Which is, you know, we can understand where the roads are
and perhaps the roads are relatively fixed.
They change over long timescales
and it's similar to the brain.
We really only create large scale connections
over long timescales.
But we can alter the traffic on the roadways really easily.
And that's similar to what we do in the brain
is that we change the way that information is flowing
to allow us to respond to our environment.
It's a great analogy.
You know I remember when I first learned of your work,
for me as somebody who studies learning and memory,
which are processes that are all about rapid updating
that to me was really mind blowing that we can now
start asking questions about dynamics of circuits
on a timescale that's much closer to the timescale
of human thought than anything we had before.
The field is definitely opening up in the kinds of
questions that can be asked now with these approaches.
And I think what you're pointing out is that conceptually
it's where we wanted to go anyway.
So if there's so much orchestration that happens
and a symphony is not a single piece,
it's not a single pattern of,
of harmony between instruments and,
and similar to the brain,
symphony of your brain is not a single pattern.
It's a very dynamically changing pattern.
In the field we often talk of have having a mental model
that we use to make predictions about what's about to happen
and really use that to mean kind of a general understanding
of what's going on.
What does that look like in terms that come from your world
of like the configuration of, of a circuit?
I think this is the most exciting area of memory research
right now I think, which is how people build and remember,
you know, and keep with them models of the world
over long periods of time.
When I think about a mental model,
I think about ideas, potential outcomes, potential events,
potential actions, and how they all depend on one another.
So when we predict what will happen next it would be,
I am currently sitting at this piece of the network
and I know that there are,
there are these possible outcomes.
And so I have to get ready for those, you know,
four possible outcomes.
But that brings this interesting question to the fore,
I think which is how does a network system in our mind
and our brain create a network model of the world outside?
It's almost like there's a reflection in the structure of
what's inside in the hardware and what we're building
in a very abstract way.
And I think that relates to some interesting work that's
coming out about the hippocampus too.
We now think of the hippocampus so much in terms of memory
but really some of the earliest work in neuroscience
on the hippocampus talked about the role of the hippocampus
in spatial navigation.
And it really exposed I think one of the deepest questions
in memory research,
or at least from the neuroscience perspective is like,
well what is the connection?
What is the connection between
spatial processing and memory?
And why is it that neurons in the hippocampus
when an animal is navigating a maze,
why is it that there's spatial specialization there?
I really do think that this notion of a mental model,
a generative model is where those,
where memory and space meet.
That makes a lot of sense.
And I think that it makes me wonder how we think about
places in the abstract, what is a cognitive place, right?
And what is, how do we map out the cognitive spaces that we,
that our mind moves within in the same way
or in a similar way to the spaces
that we physically walk in.
But I think it opens a lot up,
a lot of questions about how individuals
make their own maps, right?
And then how the way that they choose to make that map
will then affect the way that they may respond in the future
to a new context or environment.
It's really interesting.
And I think it also really speaks to the kind of pervasive
and circular role of memory, right?
Because the maps are dependent on memories,
they're built from memories and experiences.
So there's this kind of dialogue with memory creating maps,
maps creating experiences, experiences creating memories,
affecting the maps,
and really makes me think even of a question
a friend asked me recently about moods and mental health
and this feeling of like how do some people get kind of
trapped in a state of mind where all they can see
is a particular way forward.
But then something happens in the world that shifts them
to another model.
Which is actually really interesting because you can make
different maps in different physical spaces too, right?
As you walk into a new room
you make a new map of that room.
So what are the doorways basically for mental models?
And I think that often we sort of fall into this pattern of,
of thinking about memory as, as always additive,
that we always just keep gathering more information
and update what we currently have.
We don't as often speak about deleting
pieces of information or,
or perceptions or something that we thought was a,
was a useful fact but actually it's not a fact.
Memories as models or models first
and memory as part of what helps build the model
starts generating new predictions.
Exactly the kind you just brought up
or other ways of thinking about things because an efficient
model needs to let go of information, right?
I just heard a colleague of mine joking about the fact that
the principles of neuroscience textbook
keeps growing and growing.
And that if we had enough strength
in theoretical neuroscience,
if our models of neuroscience were good enough,
that book should shrink back.
We know that memory retrieval is essentially construction.
And so you don't need to store all those separate memories.
You need to construct them.
But if you have a good model,
let go of all those details and just construct them
to feel as if it's a memory.
But really all you're doing is using the model
to fill the details.
It's kind of like the matrix, you know.
Right.
Getting way out there, but,
but it is a way in which if you have the right model,
it should be an efficient one.
I'm wondering what you, what you think about what all this
means for kind of a redefinition of what memory is,
in many ways a revision or expansion of that notion.
Would you be willing to try to help me think about
how to define it?
I think it's information certainly that,
that is stored in the brain
and that can then affect our behavior.
I think getting your behavior in there somewhere
seems, seems important.
But also is it just a record of the past or is it, you know,
our perceptions of, of what happened in the past,
or how we've changed how we think about that past event
or have we deleted that event completely
and then gathered this higher level structure
or mental model that then allows us to behave differently
in the future?
So it's more alive, it's more changing.
It's more, I think it's bubbling out and gathering,
gathering and growing and becoming more flexible,
but it's also sort of bubbling in,
in the sense of deleting and removing and forgetting.
So yes, I think that what we need to add perhaps
is that changeability and perhaps the behavioral relevance
of that changeability. I don't know, what do you think?
I agree.
I think, you know, as you were speaking I was thinking
maybe the most important word
to replace there is record.
Record implies an etching of something as it was.
And maybe what we need instead is that it's a model,
it's an approximation,
it's an abstraction that can help us create the illusion of
constructing your record.
But that it's the model that's the important thing
that drives the whole thing.
Yeah, yeah I agree.
I think that's perfect. Yep.
I hope you learned something about memory.
It plays a role in shaping the changing preferences
of things we encounter in our lives.
Memory is fundamental to everything we do, to who we are,
to what we order for breakfast,
to how we think about our past, to how we plan our future.
Musician Explains One Concept in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Expert Explains One Concept in 5 Levels of Difficulty - Blockchain
Oculus' John Carmack Explains Virtual Reality in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Biologist Explains One Concept in 5 Levels of Difficulty - CRISPR
Neuroscientist Explains One Concept in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Astronomer Explains One Concept in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Laser Expert Explains One Concept in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Sleep Scientist Explains One Concept in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Physicist Explains One Concept in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Astrophysicist Explains One Concept in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Hacker Explains One Concept in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Nanotechnology Expert Explains One Concept in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Physicist Explains Origami in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Computer Scientist Explains Machine Learning in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Neuroscientist Explains Memory in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Computer Scientist Explains One Concept in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Astrophysicist Explains Black Holes in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Computer Scientist Explains Fractals in 5 Levels of Difficulty
College Professor Explains One Concept in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Quantum Computing Expert Explains One Concept in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Computer Scientist Explains One Concept in 5 Levels of Difficulty
UMass Professor Explains the Internet in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Mathematician Explains Infinity in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Theoretical Physicist Explains Time in 5 Levels of Difficulty
MIT Professor Explains Nuclear Fusion in 5 Levels of Difficulty
Harvard Professor Explains Algorithms in 5 Levels of Difficulty